Skip to main content

Does the placebo effect modulate drug bioavailability? Randomized cross-over studies of three drugs

Abstract

Background

Medication effect is the sum of its drug, placebo, and drug*placebo interaction effects. It is conceivable that the interaction effect involves modulating drug bioavailability; it was previously observed that being aware of caffeine ingestion may prolong caffeine plasma half-life. This study was set to evaluate such concept using different drugs.

Methods

Balanced single-dose, two-period, two-group, cross-over design was used to compare the pharmacokinetics of oral cephalexin, ibuprofen, and paracetamol, each described by its name (overt) or as placebo (covert). Volunteers and study coordinators were deceived as to study aim. Drug concentrations were determined blindly by in-house, high performance liquid chromatography assays. Terminal-elimination half-life (t½) (primary outcome), maximum concentration (Cmax), Cmax first time (Tmax), terminal-elimination-rate constant (λ), area-under-the-concentration-time-curve, to last measured concentration (AUCT), extrapolated to infinity (AUCI), or to Tmax of overt drug (AUCOverttmax), and Cmax/AUCI were calculated blindly using standard non-compartmental method. Covert-vs-overt effect on drug pharmacokinetics was evaluated by analysis-of-variance (ANOVA, primary analysis), 90% confidence interval (CI) using the 80.00–125.00% bioequivalence range, and percentage of individual pharmacokinetic covert/overt ratios that are outside the +25% range.

Results

Fifty, 30, and 50 healthy volunteers (18%, 10%, and 6% females, mean (SD) age 30.8 (6.2), 31.4 (6.6), and 31.2 (5.4) years) participated in 3 studies on cephalexin, ibuprofen, and paracetamol, respectively. Withdrawal rate was 4%, 0%, and 4%, respectively. Eighteen blood samples were obtained over 6, 10, and 14 h in each study period of the three drugs, respectively. ANOVA showed no significant difference in any pharmacokinetic parameter for any of the drugs. The 90% CIs for AUCT, AUCI, Cmax, AUCOverttmax, and Cmax/AUCI were within the bioequivalence range, except for ibuprofen Cmax (76.66–98.99), ibuprofen Cmax/AUCI (77.19–98.39), and ibuprofen (45.32–91.62) and paracetamol (51.45–98.96) AUCOverttmax. Out of the 126 individual covert/overt ratios, 2.0–16.7% were outside the +25% range for AUCT, 2.0–4.2% for AUCI, 25.0–44.9% for Cmax, 67.3–76.7% for AUCOverttmax, and 45.8–71.4% for Tmax.

Conclusions

This study couldn’t confirm that awareness of drug ingestion modulates its bioavailability. However, it demonstrates the trivial effect of blinding in bioequivalence studies and the extent of bio-variability that would be expected when comparing a drug product to itself.

Trial registration

ClinicalTrials.gov identifier: NCT01501747 (registered Dec 26, 2011).

Peer Review reports

Background

The placebo effect has been utilized in medical practice since antiquity and continues to be commonly used [1]. Changes over time in the placebo arm of a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled trials don’t separate the placebo effect (meaning response) [2] from methodological factors such as regression to the mean, natural course, and the Hawthorne effect. [3] Although it was once argued that if a placebo effect exists it would be of negligible importance [4], under some circumstances, the placebo effect could be clinically important [5, 6] and comparable in size to the drug effect [7,8,9,10,11]. The placebo effect may explain why generic drug products that pass rigorous bioequivalence tests are perceived as less potent (not more potent) than their more expensive branded counterparts [12, 13].

Differences between drug and placebo arms in clinical trials may represent not only drug pharmacological effect but also a drug*placebo interaction effect and thus may underestimate [7, 14] or overestimate [8] pharmacological drug effect. The possibility of a negative interaction effect may explain the clinically trivial effect of antidepressants as deduced from clinical trials [15].

It was suggested since the 1950s that the effect of false belief may countermand the effects of active drugs [16]. A neuro-imaging study showed that alcohol intoxication and expectancy have opposite effects on neuronal activation [17]. The possibility that the drug*placebo interaction effect may involve modulation of drug bioavailability has not been well explored. Theoretically, it is possible that the interaction effect may involve altering gastric emptying, intestinal transit time, or drug elimination. Previously, we conducted a 14 h bioavailability study on 22 volunteers who received caffeine described as caffeine or as placebo in a balanced randomized cross-over design. Mean plasma caffeine levels were consistently lower in the terminal part of the concentration-time curve, caffeine area-under-the-time-concentration curve (AUC) was significantly lower, and plasma caffeine terminal half-life was significantly shorter after receiving caffeine described as placebo [7], suggesting the importance of blinding in bioequivalence studies which compare generic and brand drug products and in clinical trials even with objective endpoints.

The rate and extent of drug bioavailability are commonly evaluated by maximum concentration (Cmax) and AUC to last measured concentration (AUCT) or extrapolated to infinity (AUCI), respectively, using the non-compartmental method. The rate of bioavailability can be also evaluated by first time of Cmax (Tmax), the ratio Cmax/AUCI, and AUC to Tmax of reference drug (AUCReftmax). Average bioequivalence (ABE) between a test and reference products of the same drug, the standard worldwide requirement to market generic drug products, is demonstrated if the 90% confidence interval (CI) on their Cmax, AUCT, and AUCI geometric mean ratio is in the range 80.00–125.00% [18, 19].

Several concerns have been raised regarding the ABE standards, including, using relatively wide bioequivalence limits and being unable to ensure therapeutic equivalence in all subjects; several individual pharmacokinetic ratios can fall well outside the +20% range despite demonstrating ABE. [18,19,20] Intra-subject variability is commonly estimated by intra-subject coefficient of variation (CV). Large intra-subject CV can be due to intra-drug variability (first-pass or metabolic variability, gastric emptying, etc.), intra-product variability (tablet to tablet or batch to batch), inter-product variability (generic vs. reference product), or subject-by-product interaction (i.e., the difference between products is not the same across subjects). Large intra-subject variability is especially important for narrow therapeutic index (NTI) drugs, for which individual bioequivalence (IBE) model, 75/75 rule, and intra-subject variability comparisons have been advocated [21,22,23]. The 75/75 rule requires ≥75% of individual test-reference pharmacokinetic ratios to be within +25%.

We hypothesized that the drug*placebo interaction effect may involve modulation of drug pharmacokinetics. We elected to study two over-the-counter medicines, ibuprofen and paracetamol, because of their expected familiarity to study volunteers (and hence potentially having a placebo effect) and cephalexin as a “negative control” because of its expected unfamiliarity. We were not able to confirm our previous observation in any of the three drugs. However, we used the data to explore the extent of bio-variability that can be observed when comparing a drug product to itself.

Methods

Design

Volunteers were consecutively assigned to one of three randomized, two-period, two-sequence, cross-over studies, using cephalexin, ibuprofen, or paracetamol. In each study, the volunteers received one of the three drugs twice, one time described by its name (overt) and one time described as placebo (covert). Wash-out periods and blood sampling frames were drug-specific (Table 1) and extended to ˃7 and ≥5 expected drug plasma half-life, respectively. Expected plasma half-life was about 1 h for cephalexin [24, 25], 2 h for ibuprofen [26], and 2.3 h for paracetamol [27].

Table 1 Main characteristics of three randomized, 2-period, 2-sequence, cross-over studies comparing three drugs described by their name or as placebo

Participants

We enrolled healthy (based on medical history, complete blood count, renal profile, and liver profile within 30 days), non-pregnant, non-lactating adults (age 18–60 years) with a body mass index (BMI) ≤35 kg/m2, who accepted to abstain from taking any medication (including over-the-counter) for 1 week and from smoking, alcohol, and caffeine for 48 h before and throughout each study periods. Subjects with a history of hypersensitivity to the drug to be used or with recent (one week) acute illness were excluded. For menstruating women, the study was conducted 5 to 19 days after the last menstrual period and after obtaining a negative urine pregnancy test.

The study was conducted at the King Faisal Specialist Hospital & Research Center (KFSH&RC), Riyadh from February 2012 through February 2013 after obtaining approval of the KFSH&RC Research Ethics Committee. Volunteers were compensated based on the Wage-Payment model [28] in a prorated manner. The study followed published ethical guidelines on deception use in clinical research [29,30,31,32]. A written “consent” (specific to the drug to be administered) was obtained from each volunteer; being told that the study compares the effects of tablets/capsules containing placebo to those containing the particular drug on a new serum marker, that it aims to determine how much of the observed changes in the serum marker is not related to the particular drug, and that they will each receive both the drug and placebo in a random sequence. At the completion of the three studies and after obtaining their monetary compensation, the volunteers were contacted for debriefing on the actual study aim and design and for delayed full consenting.

Procedures and interventions

The three drugs were purchased from retail pharmacies in Riyadh, Saudi Arabia. Brand name, manufacturer name, batch number, manufacturing date, and expiry date were: Keflex 500 mg, Facta Italy, 000301, 12/2010, and 12/2013 for cephalexin; Ibuprofen 400 mg, Hamol Ltd. UK, 1EE, 5/2010, and 5/2015 for ibuprofen; and Panadol 500 mg, GlaxoSmithKline, 110,216, 2/2011, and 2/2015 for paracetamol.

Few days before the study, the volunteers were reminded to abstain from smoking, alcohol, and caffeine for 48 h, from food for 10 h, and from water for one hour, and to have ≥7 h of good sleep before each study period. Compliance with study instructions was checked before administering the drugs. The drugs were administered (by MMH) with 240 ml of water at room temperature. To enhance the placebo effect, immediately before drug administration, the volunteers were individually briefed and requested to read and sign an additional “consent” document that stated: “As you know, we are doing this study to determine how much of the change in serum marker level that occurs after ingestion of (name and dose of drug) is not related to (name of drug) effect but to placebo effect. This study has two parts. One time you will take (name of drug) and one time you will take a placebo. The placebo is not known to affect the level of the marker. Today you are assigned to take (name of drug or placebo).” Overt drugs were dispensed from the original brand manufacturer bottle, whereas covert drugs were dispensed from a bottle labelled “placebo”. Fasting from food and beverages continued for 4 h post-dosing. However, volunteers were allowed 120 ml water per hour, starting one hour after drug administration. A standardized breakfast and dinner were provided 4 h and about 10 h after drug administration. Meal plans were identical in all parts of the studies. Strenuous physical activity was not permitted during study periods. Volunteers remained ambulatory or seated upright (unless deemed medically necessary) for the first four hours after drug administration and were under continuous observation regarding occurrence of adverse events and compliance with study procedures. In addition, they were directly asked about experiencing any adverse events at the time of last blood collection of each period and at the beginning of period-2.

Eighteen blood samples were obtained before and, at 0.16, 0.33, 0.5, 0.66, 0.83, 1.0, 1.25, 1.5, 1.75, 2.0, 2.5, 3.0, 3.5, 4.0, 4.5, 5.0, and 6 h after cephalexin administration, at 0.25, 0.5, 0.75, 1.0, 1.25, 1.5, 1.75, 2.0, 2.25, 2.50, 3.0, 3.5, 4.0, 5.0, 6.0, 8.0, and 10 h after ibuprofen administration, and at 0.25, 0.5, 0.75, 1.0, 1.25, 1.5, 1.75, 2.0, 2.5, 3.0, 3.5, 4.0, 6.0, 8.0, 10, 12, and 14 h after paracetamol administration. Blood samples were collected in vacutainer tubes, centrifuged (3000 rpm for 10 min) at room temperature within 15 min, and plasma samples were harvested in clean polypropylene tubes and placed immediately at –80 °C.

Drug concentrations were blindly measured by in-house, locally-validated, reversed-phase high performance liquid chromatography (HPLC) assays. [33,34,35] Limits of quantifications are shown in Table 1. Intra-assay coefficient of variation (CV, standard deviation/mean × 100) and bias (measured concentration/nominal concentration × 100) were ≤3.1% and ≤5.0% for cephalexin, ≤3.8% and ≤7.0% for ibuprofen, and ≤11.6% and ≤14.0% for paracetamol. A typical assay run included a series of 10 standard samples (calibrators), several sets of four quality control samples (concentrations at 1 and 3 times lower quantitation limit and 0.5 and 0.8–0.9 upper quantitation limit), and unknown samples. Standards and quality control samples were distributed throughout the unknown samples. Samples from the two periods of each volunteer were analyzed in the same assay run. Samples with drug concentrations greater than the upper quantitation limit were re-assayed after dilution. Drug concentrations below the lower quantitation limit were assigned zero value. Drug concentrations of missing samples were assigned the value of the average concentration of the two flanking samples in the same period.

Randomization

Three randomization schedules (one for each drug) were generated (by MMH) using an online program [36]. For each study, volunteers were block-randomized (block size = 2) to one of two sequences (drug described by its name followed by drug described as placebo and vice versa). Assignment was concealed from recruiting study coordinators and potential participants.

Deception and blinding

Study coordinators and volunteers were deceived regarding study aim and design. Volunteers were deceived in the period when they were given the drug described as placebo. To enhance deception, volunteers were requested not to reveal their assignments to the coordinators. Drug concentrations and pharmacokinetic analysis were performed blinded to assignment.

Sample size

Calculation of the sample size for each of the three studies was based on the primary analysis of a difference in drug half-life of 10%, type I error of 0.05, type II error of 0.1, and about 10% withdrawal/dropout rate. We estimated that mean and standard deviation (SD) drug half-life are, respectively, 1.0 and 0.21 h for cephalexin [24, 25], 2.0 and 0.30 h for ibuprofen [26], and 2.3 and 0.46 h for paracetamol [27]. The calculated sample size, allowing for withdrawals/dropouts, was 50 for the cephalexin study, 30 for the ibuprofen study, and 50 for the paracetamol study.

Outcome measures and analysis

The following pharmacokinetic parameters were determined using standard non-compartmental method: AUCT calculated by linear trapezoidal method, terminal rate constant (λ) calculated by linear least-squares regression analysis from a plot of natural log-transformed plasma concentration versus time curve, AUCI calculated as the sum of AUCT plus the ratio of last quantifiable plasma level/λ, AUCT/AUCI, Cmax determined directly from the observed data, Cmax/AUCI, Tmax determined directly from the observed data, t½ calculated as natural log of 2/ λ,, and AUC to Tmax of the overt drug (AUCOverttmax) calculated by linear trapezoidal method.

The primary outcome measure was t½. Secondary outcome measures were the other pharmacokinetic parameters. Outcome measures were evaluated by analysis of variance (ANOVA) after natural log-transformation, except for Tmax. The model included sequence, subjects nested within sequence, period, and intervention (covert vs overt drug administration). Mean square residual error (MSR) was used to test the significance of period and intervention effects. Subjects nested in sequence mean square was used to test the significance of sequence effect. Secondary analysis evaluated covert-overt drug ABE: the 90% CI on the difference between means of log-transformed values was determined (using MSR) and the antilog of the 90% CI limits were compared to the 80.00% and 125.00% bioequivalence limits. The null hypothesis of no placebo effect on drug bioavailability was rejected if the covert-overt drug difference was not significant at the 0.05 level. The null hypothesis of lack of bioequivalence was rejected if the 90% CI was entirely within the 80.00% to 125.00% limits. We also calculated the percentage of individual covert/overt pharmacokinetic ratios that were ˂75% or ˃125% and their mean deviation from 100%.

Pharmacokinetic calculations and statistical analysis were performed (by MMH) on a personal computer using Microsoft Excel (Version 2010) with relevant add-ins (PK Functions for Microsoft Excel, JI Usansky, A Desai, and D Tang-liu, Department of pharmacokinetics and Drug Metabolism, Allergan Irvine, CA, USA) and IBM SPSS Statistics version 21 software (SPSS Inc., Chicago, Ill, USA), respectively. Analyses were not adjusted for multiple comparisons. Two-tailed p values are reported.

Results

A total of 130 (Fig. 1) healthy volunteers participated in three two-period, two-sequence, cross-over studies that compared two single oral doses of cephalexin, ibuprofen, or paracetamol, each described by its name (overt) or as placebo (covert). As shown in Table 1, 6–18% of the volunteers per study were females. Mean (SD) age ranged from 30.8 (6.2) to 31.4 (6.6) years and mean BMI from 24.8 (3.1) to 25.2 (3.1) kg/m2. As shown in Figure 1, withdrawal rate ranged from 0% (ibuprofen) to 4% (cephalexin and paracetamol). Withdrawal reasons were personal (one volunteer withdrew before period-1 and one after period-2, refusing to provide post-study consent) or incompliance with study procedures (one volunteer smoked during the study and one did not swallow cephalexin tablet). We were able to contact 68 (54%) out of the 127 volunteers who completed the study for post-study debriefing and consenting, all gave full informed consent except one volunteer (noted above), whose data were removed from analysis. Adverse events occurred in 0% (ibuprofen) to 2% (cephalexin and paracetamol) of volunteers (Table 1); all were minor and resolved spontaneously.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Flow of participants through the study

Eighteen blood samples were obtained over 6 to 14 h in each study period with a washout interval ranging from 24 to 48 h (Table 1). There were no missing blood samples or interfering plasma peaks in any of the 3 drug assays. Baseline plasma concentrations were below the assay detection limits in all volunteers. In one volunteer in the cephalexin study, cephalexin concentration was not measurable in any sample during one study period (the volunteer, note above, admitted that he did not swallow the cephalexin tablet). The entire data of this volunteer and of the volunteer who did not provide post-study consent were not included in further analysis. Mean untransformed and natural logarithm-transformed concentration-time curve of the three drugs when administered overtly or covertly are presented in Fig. 2 and Fig. 3, respectively. The results are consistent with the results of previous studies on cephalexin [24, 25], ibuprofen [26], and paracetamol [27].

Fig. 2
figure 2

Time-concentration curves of cephalexin (a), ibuprofen (b), and paracetamol (c) described as such (blue diamonds) or as placebo (red squares). Data represent mean concentrations

Fig. 3
figure 3

Time-log-concentration curves of cephalexin (a), ibuprofen (b), and paracetamol (c) described as such (blue diamonds) or as placebo (red squares). Data represent mean natural log-transformed concentrations

We were able to calculated λ in all analyzed periods. No outlier values for AUCT, AUCI, or Cmax were identified/removed from analysis. Main pharmacokinetic parameters of the three drugs when administered overtly or covertly are summarized in Table 2. AUCT/AUCI ranged from 93% (ibuprofen) to 97% (cephalexin and paracetamol), indicating adequate sampling frames. MSR from ANOVA analysis and calculated intra-subject CV for each of the 3 drugs are presented in Table 3. The intra-subject CV ranged from 5.5% (paracetamol) to 9.5% (cephalexin) for AUCI and from 23.1% (paracetamol) to 29.8% (ibuprofen) for Cmax. There were no significant (p˃0.05) period or sequence effects in any of the three studies.

Table 2 Main pharmacokinetic parameters of three drugs described by their name (overt) or as placebo (covert)
Table 3 Bioequivalence comparison of three drugs described by their name (overt) or as placebo (covert)

No significant change in the pharmacokinetics of three drugs when described as placebo

P values from ANOVA comparing the pharmacokinetic parameters for the three drugs when each was administered overtly or covertly are presented in Table 2. There were no significant differences between the two conditions except for ibuprofen AUCOverttmax (P = 0.04). In particular, the p value for log transformed λ for the three drugs ranged from 0.052 for ibuprofen to 0.99 for cephalexin.

Average bioequivalence of three drugs when described by their names or as placebo

Table 3 summarizes the results of bioequivalent analysis comparing AUCT, AUCI, and Cmax of the three drugs when they were described by their names compared to when they were described as placebo. There was little difference between the two conditions. Absolute deviation of point estimates from 100% was ≤3.34, ≤1.46, and ≤12.89 percentage points for AUCT, AUCI, and Cmax, respectively. Further, none of the AUCT or AUCI 90% CIs failed to fall within the 80.00–125.00% bioequivalence limits and only one of the three Cmax 90% CIs barely failed to do so (76.66–98.99 for ibuprofen). The results are also depicted in Fig. 4. Power analysis revealed that the three studies had a power of ˃0.9 to show average bioequivalence for AUCT, AUCI, Cmax, and Cmax/AUCI, except for ibuprofen Cmax and Cmax /AUCI, were the power was 0.32 and 0.44, respectively.

Fig. 4
figure 4

Average bioequivalence evaluation of covert-overt cephalexin, ibuprofen, and paracetamol. Data represent point estimate (antilog of mean covert-overt difference of log-transformed values) and parametric 90% confidence interval. The shaded area indicates the area of bioequivalence (80.00% to 125.00%). a, bioequivalence evaluation of area-under-the-concentration-time curve to last quantifiable concentration (AUCT). b, bioequivalence evaluation of area-under-the-concentration-time curve extrapolated to infinity (AUCI). c, bioequivalence evaluation of maximum concentration (Cmax). d, bioequivalence evaluation of area-under-the-concentration-time curve extrapolated to overt Tmax (AUCOverttmax). e, bioequivalence evaluation of Cmax/AUCI

Table 3 and Fig. 4 also present point estimates and 90% CIs of AUCOvertmax and Cmax/AUCI of the three drugs under the overt and covert conditions. Only ibuprofen AUCOvertmax and Cmax/AUCI and paracetamol AUCOverttmax 90% CIs failed to show bioequivalence and none showed bioinequivalence.

Individual bioequivalence of three drugs when described by their names or as placebo

The percentages of individual covert/overt AUCT, AUCI, Cmax, AUCOverttmax, and Tmax ratios that are less than 0.75 or more than 1.25 are presented in Fig. 5.

Fig. 5
figure 5

Individual bioequivalence evaluation of covert-overt cephalexin, ibuprofen, and paracetamol. Data represent percentage of individual ratios that are ˂0.75 (closed bars) or ˃1.25 (open bars). a, bioequivalence evaluation of area-under-the-concentration-time curve to last quantifiable concentration (AUCT). b, bioequivalence evaluation of area-under-the-concentration-time curve extrapolated to infinity (AUCI). c, bioequivalence evaluation of maximum concentration (Cmax). d, bioequivalence evaluation of area-under-the-concentration-time curve extrapolated to overt Tmax (AUCOverttmax). e, bioequivalence evaluation of time to maximum concentration (Tmax)

About 6.9% (least square mean) of the 126 individual ratios were outside the range of 0.75 to 0.125 for AUCT with a range of 2.0% (paracetamol) to 16.7% (ibuprofen), 3.2% for AUCI with a range of 2.0% (paracetamol) to 4.2% (cephalexin), 34.4% for Cmax with a range from 25.0% (cephalexin) to 44.9% (paracetamol), 71.6% for AUCOverttmax with a range of 67.3% (paracetamol) to 76.7% (ibuprofen), and 60.2% for Tmax with a range of 45.8% (cephalexin) to 71.4% (paracetamol).

Discussion

The aim of the study was to examine the hypothesis that the drug*placebo interaction effect involves modulation of drug pharmacokinetics, namely, the knowledge that one is ingesting a drug would alter the pharmacokinetic parameters of the drug. We measured the effect of describing a drug as placebo (covert drug) on the pharmacokinetics of cephalexin, ibuprofen, and paracetamol. The three drugs were selected in part because of their known safety profiles. Ibuprofen and paracetamol were studied because of their expected familiarity (and hence ability to elicit a placebo effect) and cephalexin was used as “negative control” because of its expected unfamiliarity to study volunteers. We designed a two-period, two-sequence, cross-over study for each of the three drugs, with a 90% power to detect a 10% difference in t1/2 between overtly and covertly administered drugs. Drug concentrations were blindly determined using in-house HPLC assays and AUCT, AUCI, Cmax, Tmax, λ, t1/2, and AUCOverttmax were blindly estimated by the standard non-compartmental method. We compared the pharmacokinetic parameters under the two administration conditions using ANOVA, computed 90% CIs on the difference (covert-overt) between the means of their log-transformed values and compared them to the standard bioequivalence range of 80.00% to 125.00%, and calculated the percentages of the untransformed individual pharmacokinetic covert/overt ratios that are outside the +25% range. We found that: 1) there is no placebo effect on any of the pharmacokinetic parameters studied, 2) the two conditions of administering the drugs resulted in bioequivalent profiles, and 3) about 34.4%, 71.6%, and 60.2% of individual covert/overt ratios for Cmax, AUCOverttmax, and Tmax, respectively, were outside the +25% range.

Describing drugs as placebos doesn’t significantly alter their pharmacokinetic parameters

We found no significant differences between the covert and overt conditions in any of the studied pharmacokinetic parameters of the three drugs. This is in contrast to the results of a previous, similarly designed cross-over study on 300 mg caffeine [7], which found that mean plasma caffeine levels were consistently lower in the terminal part of the 14-h concentration-time curve, mean caffeine AUC was significantly lower, and mean plasma caffeine terminal half-life was significantly shorter when caffeine was given covertly. The reason for the discrepancy is not clear. It is possible that the results of the caffeine study were due to chance, especially because the study was exploratory, had a low power (22 subjects), and tested a novel mechanism, which are expected to increase the probability of false discovery, despite having statistically significant results [37, 38]. However, it is also possible that a placebo effect for ibuprofen and paracetamol was not successfully elicited in the current study; other outcomes (for example pain reduction) unfortunately were not examined. There are several modulators of the placebo effect, including conditioning [39], expectancy, suggestion, personality, desire for symptom change, and affective state [40].

Average bioequivalence of the three drugs under covert and overt conditions

Since no placebo effect was observed on the pharmacokinetic parameters of the three drugs, the volunteers could be considered to have received the same drug product. We thus used the data to explore the extent of bio-variability that can be observed when comparing a drug product to itself. We found that the two conditions of administering the drugs resulted in bioequivalent profiles; only one of the three Cmax 90% CIs barely failed to show bioequivalence using the strict 80%–125% bioequivalence limits. The outcome of a cross-over bioequivalence study is affected by its sample size and intra-subject variability. In retrospect, power analysis revealed that the ibuprofen study has only a 0.32 power to show bioequivalence for Cmax.

Intra-subject variability can be due to the drug substance itself (being readily affected by physiological variability of the volunteer), product quality variability, analytical variability, or unexplained random variation [41]. In a typical bioequivalence study comparing test and reference drug products, intra-subject variability includes, in addition, variability due to differences between the two products [41]. When a generic drug product is declared therapeutically equivalent to a reference product, it is expected that any difference between the two products should be no greater than the difference between two batches of the reference product. In fact, reviewing the bioequivalence studies of its approved generic products, the US Food and Drug Administration (FDA) found a mean generic-reference deviation of 3.47% for AUC and 4.29% for Cmax in one study [42] and 3.56% for AUC and 4.35% for Cmax in another [43]. Commonly, there are several marketed drug products that are linked by a chain of reference [44]; concerns have been raised that reference-bioequivalent generic products may not be bioequivalent to each other. However, secondary analysis of 120 bioequivalence studies on three immunosuppressants and six selected drugs showed a mean generic-generic deviation of 4.5% for AUCT and 5.1% for Cmax [45]. Interestingly, in the current study, we found that covert-overt deviation was ≤3.34, ≤1.46, and ≤12.89 percentage points for AUCT, AUCI, and Cmax, respectively; suggesting that most of the deviation observed in typical bioequivalence studies is not related to using two different products.

Bioequivalence studies’ guidelines of regulatory agencies (except for Health Canada (HC)) are silent regarding blinding of study volunteers [18,19,20]. Our results suggest that failure to blind would not be expected to have a negative impact; even describing the drugs as placebo did not affect their pharmacokinetic parameters.

There is disagreement among the different regulatory guidelines on the bioequivalence criteria for Cmax, whereas some require that the 90% CI should fall within the 80–125% or the 70–143% limits, others require only the point estimate to fall within the 80–125% limits [18,19,20]. Our results suggest that requiring the 90% CI to fall within the 80–125% limits may be too strict as ibuprofen reference product failed this criteria when compared to itself, despite having a relatively large sample size.

For drugs where time of onset of effect is important, the US FDA and HC recommend that the 90% CI of AUC truncated at reference product median Tmax or AUCReftmax, respectively, should be within the 80–125% limits [18,19,20]. Our results indicate that such criteria would be difficult to achieve; only one of the three 90% CIs of AUCOverttmax was within the 80–125% limits.

Individual bioequivalence of the three drugs under covert and overt conditions

It has been argued that because ABE testing focuses on differences between mean values and relatively neglects differences between variances and subject-by-product interaction, it is possible that, despite establishing ABE, a patient switched from a reference product to a generic product (or vice versa) could be over-dosed or under-dosed and that some patients may have the highest drug exposure values with the reference product and lowest exposure values with the generic product and vice versa [46]. In fact, a bioequivalent study comparing generic and reference cyclosporine products found that 38% of individual Cmax ratios and 18% of individual AUC ratios were less than 0.80 despite having 90% CI within the 80–125% limits [47].

Our findings that about 6.9%, 3.2%, and 34.4% of individual covert/overt ratios for AUCT, AUCI, and Cmax, respectively, were outside the +25% range indicate that most of the variation in individual ratios are not related to using different drug products but rather to the volunteers, drug moiety, study setting, drug assay, or random variations. Our findings are consistent with the results of previous studies [48, 49]. A simulation study (assuming inter-subject variability of 20% and intra-subject variability of 10%) found that 11.1% of the reference/reference AUC ratios fell outside the 0.80–1.25 range [48]. Further, in a fully-replicated bioequivalence study on the antiepileptic drug, lamotrigine, 3% and 18% of the generic/generic ratios and 3% and 9% of the reference/reference ratios for AUC and Cmax, respectively, were outside the 0.75–1.25 range [49].

Finally, it could be argued that some patients’ distrust of reference-bioequivalent generic products [12, 13] could be related to generic products’ different onset of action, which in turn may be related to different inactive ingredients or manufacturing processes. However, this is not likely. Onset of a drug effect is for the most part related to its pharmacodynamic characteristics and as shown in the current study, large variations in Tmax and AUCOverttmax (60.2% and 71.6% of individual ratios, respectively, were outside the ±25% range) can be observed when comparing a product to itself.

Limitations

The interpretation of the results of this study is limited by the following. 1) Intervention’s administration by an undeceived investigator may have reduced the placebo effect. 2) It is possible that the study setting and the drugs used were not conducive to elicit adequate placebo effect; thus the finding of no difference in pharmacokinetic parameters between the overt and covert conditions may not be related to the placebo effect not modulating drug bioavailability but rather to failure to induce a placebo effect. 3) The protocol-defined aim of the study was to examine if the drug*placebo effect involves modulating drug bioavailability; thus the findings regarding bioequivalence of drug products with themselves are based on post hoc analysis. 4) Our study was not designed to partition intra-subject variability into its various components. Thus, it is not clear how much of the observed variability is due to the drug products themselves (i.e. to drug product quality variability rather than to the drug moiety, random error, etc.).

Conclusions

This study couldn’t confirm that awareness of drug ingestion can modulate its bioavailability. Although this may be due to inability to elicit adequate placebo effect, the results cast doubt on the concept that the drug*placebo interaction effect may involve modulating drug pharmacokinetics through mechanisms such as altering gastric emptying, intestinal transit time, or drug elimination. On the other hand, the study demonstrates that most of the generic-reference deviation observed in typical bioequivalence studies may not be related to using different products but is inherent in study design and setting, that failure to blind subjects in bioequivalence studies may not negatively impact validity of the results, that the 80–125% bioequivalence limits for Cmax and AUCOverttmax 90% CI may be too strict, and that considerable intra-subject variability in Cmax, Tmax, and AUCReftmax would be expected even when comparing a drug product to itself.

Abbreviations

ABE:

Average bioequivalence

ANOVA:

Analysis of variance

AUCT :

Area-under-the-concentration-time-curve to last measured concentration

AUCI :

Area-under-the-concentration-time-curve extrapolated to infinity

AUCOverttmax :

Area-under-the-concentration-time-curve to time of maximum concentration (Tmax) of drug given overtly

AUCReftmax :

Area-under-the-concentration-time-curve to time of maximum concentration (Tmax) of reference product

BMI:

Body mass index

CI:

Confidence interval

Cmax :

Maximum concentration

CV:

Coefficient of variation

FDA:

Food and Drug Administration

HC:

Health Canada

HPLC:

High performance liquid chromatography

IBE:

Individual bioequivalence

KFSH&RC:

King Faisal Specialist Hospital and Research Center

MSR:

Mean square residual error from ANOVA

SD:

Standard deviation

Tmax :

Time of maximum concentration

Λ:

Terminal elimination rate constant

References

  1. Tilburt JC, Emanuel EJ, Kaptchuk TJ, Curlin FA, Miller FG. Prescribing “placebo treatments”: Results of national survey of US internists and rheumatologists. BMJ. 2008;337:a1983.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Moerman DE, Jonas WB. Deconstructing the placebo effect and finding the meaning response. Ann Intern Med. 2002;136(6):471–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Kienle GS, Kiene H. The powerful placebo effect: fact or fiction? J Clin Epidemiol. 1997;50(12):1311–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Hróbjartsson A, Gøtzsche PC. Is the placebo powerless? An analysis of clinical trials comparing placebo with no treatment. N Engl J Med. 2001;344(21):1594–602.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. McRae C, Cherin E, Yamazaki TG, Diem G, Vo AH, Russell D, et al. Effects of perceived treatment on quality of life and medical outcomes in a double-blind placebo surgery trial. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2004;61(4):412–20.

  6. Preference Collaborative Review Group. Patients’ preferences within randomised trials: systematic review and patient level meta-analysis. BMJ. 2008;337:a1864.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Hammami MM, Al Gaai E, Alvi S, Hammami MB. Interaction between drug and placebo effect: across over balanced placebo design. Trials. 2010;11:110.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  8. Hammami MM, Hammami S, Al-Swayeh R, Al-Gaai E, Farah FA, De Padua SJ. Drug*placebo interaction effect may bias clinical trials interpretation: hybrid balanced placebo and randomized placebo-controlled design. BMC Med Res Methodol. 2016;16:166.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Krogsbøll LT, Hróbjartsson A, Gøtzsche PC. Spontaneous improvement in randomised clinical trials: meta-analysis of three-armed trials comparing no treatment, placebo and active intervention. BMC Med Res Methodol. 2009;9:1.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Sayette MA, Breslin FC, Wilson GT, Rosenblum GD. An evaluation of the balanced placebo design in alcohol administration research. Addict behave. 1994;19(3):333–42.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  11. Schneider R, Grüner M, Heiland A, Keller M, Kujanora Z, Peper M, et al. Effects of expectation and caffeine on arousal, well-being, and reaction time. Int J Behav Med. 2006;13(4):330–9.

  12. Dunne SS, Dunne CP. What do people really think of generic medicines? A systematic review and critical appraisal of literature on stakeholder perceptions of generic drugs. BMC Med. 2015;13:173.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  13. Piguet V, D’Incau S, Besson M, Desmeules J, Cedraschi C. Prescribing generic medication in chronic musculoskeletal pain patients: an issue of representations, trust, and experience in a Swiss cohort. PLoS One. 2015;10(8):e0134661.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Lund K, Vase L, Petersen GL, Jensen TS, Finnerup NB. Randomised controlled trials may underestimate drug effects: balanced placebo trial design. PLoS One. 2014;9(1):e84104.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Kirsch I, Moore TJ, Scoboria A, Nicholls SS. The emperor's new drugs: An analysis of antidepressant medication data submitted to the U.S. Food and Drug Administration. Prevention & Treatment. 2002; 5(1): ArtID 23. http://psycnet.apa.org/journals/pre/5/1/23a/. Accessed 1 May 2017.

  16. Wolf S. The pharmacology of placebos. Pharmacol Rev. 1959;11:689–704.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Gundersen H, Specht K, Grüner R, Ersland L, Hugdahl K. Separating the effects of alcohol and expectancy on brain activation: an fMRI working memory study. NeuroImage. 2008;42(4):1587–96.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Davit B, Braddy AC, Conner DP, Yu LX. International guidelines for bioequivalence of systemically available orally administered generic drug products: a survey of similarities and differences. APPS J. 2013;15(4):974–90.

    Google Scholar 

  19. Chen M-L, Shah VP, Crommelin DJ, Shargel L, Bashaw D, Bhatti M, et al. Harmonization of regulatory approaches for evaluating therapeutic equivalence and interchangeability of multisource drug products: workshop summary report. AAPS J. 2011;13(4):556–64.

  20. Kaushal N, Singh SK, Gulati M, Vaidya Y, Kaushik M. Study of regulatory requirements for the conduct of bioequivalence studies in US, Europe, Canada, India, ASEAN and SADC countries: Impact on generic drug substitution. J App Pharm Sci. 2016;6(4):206–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Guidance for industry. Statistical approaches to establishing bioequivalence. US DHHS, FDA, CDER; 2001. https://www.fda.gov/downloads/Drugs/Guidances/ucm070244.pdf. Accessed 1 May 2017.

  22. Hsuan FC. Some statistical considerations on the FDA draft guidance for individual bioequivalence. Stat Med. 2000;19(20):2879–84.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Buehler G. History of bioequivalence for critical dose drugs. http://www.fda.gov/downloads/%20AdvisoryCommittees/%E2%80%A6/UCM209319.pdf. Accessed 1 May 2017.

  24. Lecaillon J, Hirtz J, Schoeller J, Humbert G, Vischer W. Pharmacokinetic comparison of cefroxadin (CGP 9000) and cephalexin by simultaneous administration to humans. Antimicrob Agents Chemother. 1980;18(4):656–60.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  25. Deppermann K, Lode H, Hoffken G, Tschink G, Kalz C, Koeppe P. Influence of ranitidine, piprenzepine, and aluminum magnesium hydroxide on the bioavailability of various antibiotics, including amoxicillin, cephalexin, doxycycline, and amoxicillin-clavulanic avid. Antimicrob Agents Chemother. 1989;33(11):1901–7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Schettler T, Paris S, Pellett M, Kidner S, Wilkinson D. Comparative pharmacokinetics of two fast-dissolving oral ibuprofen formulations and a regular-release ibuprofen tablet in healthy volunteers. Clin Drug Investig. 2001;21(1):73–8.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  27. Portoles A, Puerro M, Terleira A, Rodriguez A, Caturla MC, Fernandez N, et al. A new high-absorption-rate paracetamol 500-mg formulation: a comparative bioavailability study in healthy volunteers. Curr Ther Res Clin Exp. 2003;64(7):401–11.

  28. Dickert N, Grady C. What’s the price for a research subject? Approaches to payment for research participation. N Engl J Med. 1999;341(3):198–203.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Miller FG, Wendler D, Swartzman LC. Deception in research on the placebo effect. PLoS Med. 2005;2(9):e262. doi:10.1371/journal.pmed.0020262.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  30. United States Department of Health and Human Services. Institutional Review Board Guidebook (Chapter V): Biomedical and Behavioral Research – An Overview. https://archive.hhs.gov/ohrp/irb/irb_chapter5.htm. Accessed 1 May 2017.

  31. American Psychological Association. Ethical Principles of Psychologists and Code of Conduct (including 2010 and 2016 Amendments). http://www.apa.org/ethics/code/. Accessed 1 May 2017.

  32. Council for International Organizations of Medical Sciences, International Ethical Guidelines for Health-related Research Involving Humans. Council for International Organizations of Medical Sciences (CIOMS). http://cioms.ch/ethical-guidelines-2016/WEB-CIOMS-EthicalGuidelines.pdf. Accessed 1 May 2017.

  33. Hussein RF, Hammami MM. Determination of cephalexin level and stability in human plasma by fully validated rapid HPLC analysis. WJPPS. 2014;3(12):20–31.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  34. Alvi SN, Yusuf A, Al Gaai E, Hammami MM. Rapid determination of ibuprofen concentration in human plasma by high performance liquid chromatography. WJPPS. 2014;3(8):1767–77.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  35. Alswayeh R, Alvi SN, Hammami MM. Rapid determination of acetaminophen levels in human plasma by high performance liquid chromatography. Am J PharmTech Res. 2016;6(3):710–9.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  36. Randomization.com. Dallal GE. http://www.randomization.com/. Accessed 1 May 2017.

  37. Nuzzo R. Scientific method: statistical errors. Nature. 2014;506(7487):150–2. doi:10.1038/506150a.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Sterne JAC, Smith GD. Sifting the evidence—what's wrong with significance tests? BMJ. 2001;322(7280):226–31.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  39. Lilford RJ, Braunholtz DA. Is the placebo powerless: Correspondence to the Editor. N Engl J Med. 2001;345(17):1277–8.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Siegel S. Explanatory mechanisms for placebo effects: Pavlovian conditioning. In: Guess H, Kleinman A, Kusek J, Engel L, editors. The Science of the Placebo: Toward an interdisciplinary agenda. London, England: BMJ Publishing Group; 2002. p. 133–57.

    Google Scholar 

  41. Jiang W, Makhlouf F, Schuirmann DJ, Zhang X, Zheng N, Conner D, et al. A bioequivalence approach for generic narrow therapeutic index drugs: evaluation of the reference-scaled approach and variability comparison criterion. AAPS J. 2015;17(4):891–901.

  42. Henney JE. Review of generic bioequivalence studies. JAMA. 1999;282(21):1995.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Davit BM, Nwakama PE, Buehler GJ, Conner DP, Haider SH, Patel DT, et al. Comparing generic and innovator drugs: a review of 12 years of bioequivalence data from the United States Food and Drug Administration. Ann Pharmacother. 2009;43(10):1583–97.

  44. Birkett DJ. Generics-equal or not? Aust Prescr. 2003;26(4):85–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Yu Y, Teerenstra S, Neef C, Burger D, Maliepaard M. Investigation into the interchangeability of generic formulations using immunosuppressants and a broad selection of medicines. Eur J Clin Pharmacol. 2015;71(8):979–90.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  46. Tóthfalusi L, Endrényi L, Chow SC. Statistical and regulatory considerations in assessments of interchangeability of biological drug products. Eur J Health Econ. 2014;15(Suppl 1):S5–11.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Johnston A. Equivalence and interchangeability of narrow therapeutic index drugs in organ transplantation. Eur J Hosp Pharm Sci Pract. 2013;20(5):302–7.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  48. Yim DS. Simulation of the AUC changes after generic substitution in patients. J Korean Med Sci. 2009;24(1):7–12.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  49. Ting TY, Jiang W, Lionberger R, Wong J, Jones JW, Kane MA, et al. Generic lamotrigine versus brand-name Lamictal bioequivalence in patients with epilepsy: A field test of the FDA bioequivalence standard. Epilepsia. 2015;56(9):1415–24.

Download references

Acknowledgments

Special thanks to participants of the study for their dedication and contribution to research. The superb assistance by the research staff at the Clinical Studies and Empirical Ethics Department is gratefully acknowledged.

Funding

This study was supported by a research grant (RAC 2101105) from the King Faisal Specialist Hospital & Research Center, Riyadh, Saudi Arabia to MMH. The funder had no role in the design and conduct of the study; in the collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; or in the preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript.

Availability of data and materials

The dataset supporting the conclusions of this article is available upon request from MMH.

Authors’ contributions

MMH had full access to all of the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis. Study concept and design: MMH. Acquisition, analysis, or interpretation of data: All authors. Drafting of the manuscript: MMH. Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: All authors. Statistical analysis: MMH. Administrative, technical, or material support: AY, FSS, RH, RAS. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Ethics approval and consent to participate

A written “consent” (specific to the drug to be administered) was obtained from each volunteer; being told that the study compares the effects of tablets/capsules containing placebo to those containing the particular drug on a new serum marker, that it aims to determine how much of the observed changes in the serum marker is not related to the particular drug, and that they will each receive both the drug and placebo in a random sequence. At the completion of the three studies and after obtaining their monetary compensation, the volunteers were contacted for debriefing on the actual study aim and design and for delayed full consenting.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Muhammad M Hammami.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Hammami, M.M., Yusuf, A., Shire, F.S. et al. Does the placebo effect modulate drug bioavailability? Randomized cross-over studies of three drugs. J Negat Results BioMed 16, 10 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12952-017-0075-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12952-017-0075-2

Keywords